L focus: effects on perceptual quality. PubMed ID:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21366473 J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 14, 18802. doi: 10.10370096-1523.14.two.188 Eckstein, M. P., Abbey, C. K., Pham, B. T., and Shimozaki, S. S. (2004). Perceptual mastering through optimization of attentional weighting: human versus optimal Bayesian learner. J. Vis. 4, 1006019. doi: ten.11674.12.3 Fischer, B. J., and Pena, J. L. (2011). Owl’s Mirin biological activity behavior and neural representation predicted by Bayesian inference. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1061066. doi: 10.1038nn.2872 Fiser, J., and Aslin, R. N. (2001). Unsupervised statistical understanding of higher-order spatial structures from visual scenes. Psychol. Sci. 12, 49904. doi: ten.11111467-9280. 00392 Fiser, J., and Aslin, R. N. (2002). Statistical finding out of new visual feature combinations by infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 158225826. doi: 10.1073pnas.232472899 Fiser, J., Berkes, P., Orban, G., and Lengyel, M. (2010). Statistically optimal perception and understanding: from behavior to neural representations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 11930. doi: ten.1016j.tics.2010.01.003 Franko, E., Seitz, A. R., and Vogels, R. (2010). Dissociable neural effects of long-term stimulus-reward pairing in macaque visual cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1425439. doi: ten.1162jocn.2009.21288 Friston, K. (2012). The history of the future with the Bayesian brain. Neuroimage 62, 1230233. doi: 10.1016j.neuroimage.2011.ten.004 Ganguli, D., and Simoncelli, E. P. (2010). Implicit encoding of prior probabilities in optimal neural populations. Adv. Neural Inf. Procedure. Syst. 23, 65866. Geisler, W. S., and Perry, J. S. (2009). Contour statistics in all-natural photos: grouping across occlusions. Vis. Neurosci. 26, 10921. doi: ten.1017S0952523808080875 Gekas, N., Chalk, M., Seitz, A., and Seri , P. (2013). Complexity and specificity of experimentally induced expectations in motion perception. J. Vis. 13, pii: eight. doi: 10.116713.4.8 Geldard, F. A. (1976). The saltatory effect in vision. Sens. Processes 1, 776.
A brain-computer interface (BCI) method provides its customers with control channels which might be independent of the brain’s output channels (i.e., the peripheral nervous technique and muscles) (Wolpaw et al., 2002). Such systems may be utilized as a means for communications and restoration of motor functions (by means of a neuroprosthesis) for individuals with motor issues including amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) and spinal cord injury, andor folks in the persistent locked-in state (LIS). It may also be employed as a neurorehabilitation tool to improve motor andor cognitive overall performance of such men and women. A typical BCI method consists of five stages (see Figure 1): brain-signal acquisition, preprocessing, function extractionselection, classification, and application interface. In the 1st brain-signal acquisition stage, appropriate signals are acquired utilizing an acceptable brain-imaging modality. Since the acquired signals are ordinarily weak and contain noises (physiological and instrumental) and artifacts, preprocessing is required, which is the second stage. In the third stage, some useful information so known as “features” are extracted. These options, in the fourth stage, are classified working with a appropriate classifier. Ultimately, in the fifthstage, the classified signals are transmitted to a computer or other external devices for producing the preferred handle commands to the devices. In neurofeedback applications, a real-time display of brain activity is desirable, which enables self-regulation of brain functions. Figure 1 depicts a sch.